Sport Psychology and the Relationship
between Arousal and Stress
Tom Pickett 325880
fao; Scott Reed
learning objectives:
LO1 - Demonstrate knowledge and
critical understanding of the theoretical underpinning of psychology in sports
performance.
LO2 - Demonstrate knowledge of the main methods of
enquiry in arousal and its effects on sports performance.
LO3 - Communicate effectively information and
arguments on the theories of stress and apply the theories to optimising sports
performance.
It is believed by many that sport is entirely based upon physical ability and fitness, however there are many arguments to suggest that it is completely psychological or a balance of either. Gardner, D, (2015) stated that sports performance depends 100% on psychological dependence and that our thoughts influence our actions. As everyone is individual, no single person has the same beliefs or personality to apply this.
Hollander, (1971) states that personality is ‘the sum total of an individual’s characteristics which make him/her unique’. 3 theories of personality have been analysed in this piece: Martens schematic view, psychodynamic theory and the trait theory.
Hollander, (1971) states that personality is ‘the sum total of an individual’s characteristics which make him/her unique’. 3 theories of personality have been analysed in this piece: Martens schematic view, psychodynamic theory and the trait theory.
Figure 1 (Martens Schematic View) |
Figure 2 (Psychodynamic Theory) |
This theory is less simple to Martens schematic view by including the unconscious part, however is more realistic as MSV suggests that every decision or action is a conscious thought. That being said, unlike MSV, the psychodynamic theory does not consider role related behaviours, where the individual adapts to changes in roles, demands and environments.
Figure 3 (Trait Theory) |
Motivation is the drive of an individual to start and then continue with the activities relating to their sport. Simply defined; it is the ability to initiate and persist at a task. (Taylor. J Ph.D.) Motivation can either be intrinsic, which is the individuals ‘want or need’ to participate, or can be extrinsic, where the individual strives to achieve tangible and intangible rewards.
Motivation fits into one of three general theories that parallel the traits of personality (Weinberg, 2015) the first is the Trait centred view or ‘Participation centred view’. This is where behaviour is a result of a combination of someone’s characteristics and personality. The individual’s intrinsic motivation affects the success of the athlete. However environmental influences are not taken into account.
Gledhill (2007) says this theory is far too simplistic. The situation centred view suggests motivation is influenced by situations the athlete is in. E.g. an athlete may be lazy during training, yet fully motivated and involved in competitive game situations. This view takes into consideration the task given however still does not consider outside environmental values such as weather, crowds or location. Gledhill (2007) makes the point that how is it possible to measure the athlete’s motivation if the situation is unfavourable? E.g. being behind in a game of tennis. The interactional view is the best and the most widely acknowledged. It takes into account both personality and situations as factors that influence motivation. Such as weather, standard of play and opposition, quality of facilities, coach’s expectations and even self esteem. Another example of this could be expectations of large crowds and peers. Weinberg, (2014) stated that an experiment was undertaken with a pair of swimmers, where they were asked to swim without peers present and then again in front of peers. The swimmers were psychologically influenced to swim faster by the presence of their peers and showed faster times.
Arousal can be simply referred to as a state of physiological alertness and anticipation of the autonomic nervous system, which prepares the body. The name of the nervous system tells us that it is not voluntarily controllable. For example we are not in control of our heart rate, blood pressure or breathing rates. Arousal ranges on a continuum from deep sleep to extreme excitement.
Arousal can have positive and
negative effects in performance, dependant on the athlete, or game situation. An
athletes’ arousal is also fully changeable and is never constant during an
event, reliant to certain situations they are involved in.. This can be
explored through following theories.
An early theory that links
arousal and performance, it suggests that as arousal increases, so does
performance. It was developed by Hull (1951) and Spence (1956). The theory
works on the equation:
Performance = skill x
arousal. E.g. a basketball player that has recently learnt how to successfully
‘slam dunk’ and applies it to a game situation. If they were to score with this
method, their arousal levels would increase.
Spence and Spence (1966)
explored 25 studies on arousal and performance, and all but 4 positively
correlated with this Theory.
(Gadsdon. S, 2001)
explains that arousal can improve performance much like the drive
theory, however, only to a certain point. After this point, as arousal
increases further, performance worsens. (Gadsdon. S, 2001)
E.g. A basketball player that
has missed a 3-point shot in a game, may keep on attempting to score 3-point shots
however may fail to score at all, increasing frustration and possible anger
levels. At this point, the athlete would be questioning themselves why they are
failing to score the shot; decreasing concentration levels, resulting in
decreases in performance, and high levels of frustration and anger.
Catastrophe
Theory: (see figure 6)
Figure 6 (Catastrophe Theory) |
An individual’s interpretation
of their arousal, influences his or her performance. some athletes need
extremely high levels of arousal and use it in a positive way rather than
negatively. I.e. it is common for weightlifters to get angry before lifting to
perform well. Here is an example of a weight lifter psyching up. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=RrW-qSQBzwc
An athlete to use this high
arousal in a positive way to perform better is described as Reversal theory. Other sports
such as rugby, an increasing number of teams perform threatening dances to psych
themselves up and to scare the opponents.
Athletes use very high or low
levels of arousal depending on the sport. As people are individual and have
their own requirements to perform better, they have Individual zones of optimal functioning. (IZOF). As weightlifters often require high levels of arousal, sports
such as snooker and archery often require a calm and collective environment to
allow the players to feel more relaxed and allows them to concentrate more on
the task. However tennis players require a calm environment to play, their
arousal levels can be moderately high as they need to use speed and strength to
match the other player. Figure7 shows different
athletes optimal zones and what levels of arousal they perform best at.
Figure 7 (Individual Zone of Optimal Functioning (IZOF) ) |
High levels of arousal, anxiety, stress and nervousness, bought on by high pressure situations can often cause athletes to ‘choke’. This is an unfortunate situation to be in, and can have severe results. The most common example is footballers that have to take penalties during a penalty shoot out. Examples of this can be seen here. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7jG1TRFIj6c
Stress can be described as ‘A pattern of negative
physiological states and psychological responses occurring in situations where
people perceive threats to their well being, which they may be unable to meet’
(Lazarus and Follerman, 1984)
In order to optimise sports performance, the following two
theories will be argued and compared.
figure 8 (General Adaptations Syndrome) |
Figure 9 (Transactional Model) |
describes this as the primary appraisal. The secondary appraisal makes the athlete think whether the situation is going to benefit them or not. Unlike the GAS theory, this theory allows the athlete to perceive the stress as ‘eustress’. In certain situations, it enhances performance. (Honeybourne, J. 2003)
Furthermore, Eysenk, M stated:
‘Individuals differences can be used as criticism of the GAS.
Individuals perception of stress and their reaction to it varies’. (2005)
References:
Adams, M, et al.
(2010). Psychology of sports performance. In: Pearson BTEC Level 3
National Sport Student Book. p4. Chapter 17
Eysenck, M
(2005). Psychology for AS Level. 3rd ed. Taylor & Francis.
p144.
Gadsdon, S
(2001). Psychology and Sport: Heinemann. p22.
Gardner, D.
(2015). How Much of Your Sport is Mental?. Available:
http://www.maxpreps.com/news/NSRCCf85gU68n1uBllox3w/how-much-of-your-sport-is-mental.htm.
Last accessed 5th Mar 2017.
Hill, G
(2001). A Level Psychology Through Diagrams. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford
University Press. p260.
Honeybourne, J
(2003). BTEC National Sport Textbook: Development, Coaching and Fitness.
Nelson Thornes. p97.
Horikawa, M &
Yagi, A: The Relationships among Trait Anxiety, State Anxiety and the Goal
Performance of Penalty Shoot-Out by University Soccer Players. PLoS One, 7(4)
e35727
Scott, A.
(2012). Lazarus and Folkman Transactional model. Available:
https://www.slideshare.net/Psyccounting/lazarus-and-folkman-transactional-model.
Last accessed 14 Mar 2017.
Taylor, J Ph.D.
(2009). Sports: What Motivates Athletes?. Available:
https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-power-prime/200910/sports-what-motivates-athletes.
Last accessed 21st Feb 2017.
Weinburg et al
(2014). Foundations of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 6th Ed: Cloth
Pass/Kycd.
_______________________________________________________________
Video sources:
YouTube:
Top 10 Worst Penalty Kicks Fail (2nd Nov 2014) - https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7jG1TRFIj6c
Beckham Red Card v Argentina 1998 (26th Apr
2012) -